Skip to main content
Open Access

School Tracking and Mental Health

Abstract

To understand how the type of education affects long-term mental health, we examine the effects of a comprehensive school reform on mental health–related hospitalizations and deaths. The reform postponed the tracking of students into vocational and academic schools from age 11 to age 16, thus affecting the set of peers and the curriculum to which these students were exposed. The reform was implemented gradually across Finnish municipalities between 1972 and 1977. We use difference-in-differences variation and administrative data. Our overall results show no discernible effects on mental health–related hospitalizations or deaths, but heterogeneity analysis shows an adverse effect on hospitalizations due to depression for females from highly educated families.

I.  Introduction

Education leads to monetary (Angrist and Krueger 1991) and nonmonetary (Oreopoulos and Salvanes 2011) gains at the individual level. The potential positive effect on health is a crucial part of the nonmonetary return provided by education. The positive correlation between education and health is well established (Cutler and Lleras-Muney 2008). However, quasi-experimental evidence using natural policy experiments on the causal link between education and health outcomes still remains inconclusive (Galama, Lleras-Muney, and van Kippersluis 2018).

We advance the understanding of the education-health relationship by studying the effect of a change at the age at which students are split between academic and vocational education on mental health in adulthood.1 So far, the literature has focused only on physical health. Therefore, in this study, we examine the effects of education on mental health. The lack of evidence on the effects on mental health outcomes is a salient gap, since mental health is an increasingly important domain of health, especially in the developed countries (Frank and McGuire 2000; Layard 2013). Depressive disorders are a leading and often underestimated cause of the global disease burden (Vigo, Thornicroft, and Atun 2016). For example, depressive disorders account for 12% of total years lived with disability, and depression is the largest contributor to the disease burden attributable to nonfatal health outcomes (Üstün and Chatterji 2001; Whiteford et al. 2013). Mental health problems also lead to substantial indirect costs, such as absenteeism and productivity losses at work (Bubonya, Cobb-Clark, and Wooden 2017). Additionally, mental health problems increase the risk of poor physical health (Sareen et al. 2006).

Moreover, most of the literature studies only the effects of one additional year of education on health, whereas other relevant aspects of education, such as how long students are exposed to a common curriculum before being split between academic and vocational tracks, might also have an effect on health, especially on mental health. Indeed, school tracking fundamentally affects the set of peers to which students are exposed as well as the type of skills they acquire and the degree of competition they face in the classroom. Peer effects and exposure to competition are potential drivers of mental well-being. Understanding whether and how changes in school tracking affect mental health provides insights into the mechanisms through which education relates to health. Many European countries have implemented comprehensive schooling reforms since the end of the Second World War to delay the age at which students are selected into different tracks (Brunello, Giannini, and Ariga 2007).2 The primary motivation behind such reform policies was the belief that early tracking systems were unfair to pupils from disadvantaged backgrounds (Jones, Roemer, and Rosa Dias 2014).

To identify the effect of school tracking on mental health–related hospitalizations and deaths, we use difference-in-differences variation triggered by the Finnish comprehensive school reform, which was implemented gradually across Finnish municipalities during 1972–77.3 The phase-in of the reform offers plausibly exogenous variation in the tracking age, and its occurrence as far back as the 1970s allows us to identify long-run health effects. Key to our identification strategy is the fact that the gradual rollout was orthogonal to the incidence of mental health problems before the reform. Our evidence confirms that the municipalities that were treated first were not different in terms of mental health from those that were treated later. Moreover, the reform postponed the tracking of students into vocational and academic schools from age 11 to age 16 without affecting the length of compulsory education. Thus, the reform provides a unique opportunity to study the effect of increasing the age for school tracking, holding fixed the number of years of compulsory education.

To identify the effects of this reform on mental health, we use administrative data for the Finnish population born in the 1960s. We have access to complete registers on suicides, mental health–related deaths and hospitalizations, and all-cause mortality from the late 1960s to 2013. The registers include all hospital admissions related to mental health disorders in Finland. Using the gradual rollout of the comprehensive school reform across regions and over time, we estimate difference-in-differences models to identify the effects of the reform on mental health outcomes by the age of 45.

Overall, we do not find significant effects on mental health–related hospitalizations or deaths. This average null result is precisely estimated. We contribute to the debate on whether investing in people’s education is an effective way to improve their health as well, which ultimately hinges on whether the correlation between education and health is causal (Jones, Roemer, and Rosa Dias 2014). Our work brings further evidence to previous research finding negligible causal effects (Meghir, Palme, and Simeonova 2018). It matters because, as reviewed by Galama, Lleras-Muney, and van Kippersluis (2018), the type of education received seems to affect health behaviors more than the length of education. Hence, our study, in theory, should be better able to detect effects on health than previous studies relying on compulsory-schooling reforms that identify the effect of length of education on health. However, we still do not find an economically significant causal effect.

II.  The Education Reform and Its Expected Effects on Mental Health

A.  The Structure of the Finnish Education System before and after the Reform

Finland had a selective two-track school system until the 1970s. The reform replaced the old two-track system with a uniform comprehensive school system (Somerkivi 1982). Figure 1 describes the structures of both systems (see Sahlberg 2014).

Figure 1. 
Figure 1. 

Structure of the Finnish education system before and after the comprehensive school reform in the 1970s. ISCED = International Standard Classification of Education.

The reform postponed the tracking age from 11 to 16 years. Both before and after the reform, school starts at age 7 and is compulsory until age 16. In the old system, pupils were taught together in the same class for only four years, from age 7 to age 11. Then they were placed into academic or vocational tracks for the remaining five years. In contrast, in the new system, there is an almost uniform curriculum for all nine years, until age 16.4 At its core, the reform significantly affected the composition of the peers to whom pupils were exposed between ages 11 and 16.5 There were no systematic changes in classroom size, gender composition of classes, or teacher quality due to the reform.

The comprehensive school reform was rolled out gradually across Finnish municipalities over the period 1972–77 (fig. 2). The timing of the reform in the different municipalities was decided by the National Board of Education (NBE). Municipalities made suggestions regarding the timing of the reform, but it was the NBE that finally approved and ratified them. Municipalities were in charge of the practical implementation of the reform (in collaboration with the surrounding municipalities). There is spatial correlation in the timing of the reform, because the NBE wanted to make sure that within larger areas students would get equal opportunities to move to secondary education (Somerkivi 1982). Nevertheless, there is meaningful variation in exposure to the new comprehensive school system, both across birth cohorts and across municipalities (see table 1 below).6 This variation provides a quasi-experimental research setting.

Figure 2. 
Figure 2. 

Adoption of the school reform during the period 1972–77.

Previous research on the Finnish comprehensive school reform has shown that it decreased the intergenerational correlation between earnings of fathers and sons (Pekkarinen, Uusitalo, and Pekkala Kerr 2009). The reform also slightly improved the verbal and mathematical test scores of boys belonging to low socioeconomic backgrounds (Pekkala Kerr, Pekkarinen, and Uusitalo 2013). In addition, the reform increased the gender difference in the probability of choosing an academic track and obtaining tertiary education (Pekkarinen 2008). Finally, Ravesteijn et al. (2017) study the effect of the reform on all-cause mortality. For males, using an 11% random sample of the Finnish population, they find occasionally negative or positive effects, depending on the subsample. For females, they do not find any effect on mortality. Using total population data, we focus on the effects of the reform on mental health.

B.  Expected Effects on Mental Health

A key feature of the Finnish reform is that it did not affect the length of compulsory schooling. Thus, the reform did not change the minimum school-leaving age of 16. This allows us to focus on examining the effects of change in the age of tracking on mental health outcomes while keeping the number of years of compulsory education fixed.

School tracking reform may positively affect education and income in adulthood. In turn, better human capital and availability of financial resources may improve health (Galama, Lleras-Muney, and van Kippersluis 2018). For this reason, we analyze the potential mediating role of education and income on mental health in section VI.

Besides education and income effects, other potential mechanisms are also at play. Interactions with peers in school during childhood and adolescence are important determinants of mental health outcomes in adulthood (WHO 2014). In theory, the predicted effects of the reform on health outcomes, and especially on mental health, are ambiguous. After the reform, students aged 11–16 now have a common set of peers rather than peers from their specific track only. Low-achieving students, who would have been assigned to the vocational track, are being exposed to higher-achieving peers in the postreform system. This change is expected to improve education and related economic outcomes for low-achieving students, which in turn may improve their health outcomes in the long run. It is also possible that low-achieving students will start adopting and imitating health behaviors of their higher-achieving peers. Conversely, higher-achieving students may be adversely affected by being exposed to lower-achieving peers.7

The comprehensive school reform also affected the ability ranking in the classroom. Vocational track students who scored high in the ability ranking in the prereform system are now, on average, ranked lower in comprehensive-school classrooms. Cicala, Fryer, and Spenkuch (2018) have shown that students’ academic achievements and disruptive behaviors depend on their ordinal ranking among their peers. Moreover, Elsner and Isphording (2018) provide evidence that a student’s ordinal ability ranking in a high school cohort is an important determinant of engaging in risky behaviors (such as smoking, drinking, and proneness to physical fights). This evidence suggests that pupils who would have been tracked into vocational schooling without the reform could have lower health, especially poor mental health, after the reform. It contradicts the positive effect of being exposed to higher-achieving peers.

Moreover, the fact that students in the postreform system follow a common curriculum from age 11 to age 16, rather than a track-specific curriculum, also means that, after the reform, students in a given classroom are less homogeneous. This makes it more difficult for teachers to tailor their pedagogical approaches to a more heterogeneous mix of students, thereby resulting in negative education and health outcomes (Betts 2011). Students’ achievement may be better when they are surrounded by peers with similar characteristics. Indeed, the Finnish experience suggests that mixed-ability groups led to learning difficulties; disruptive behaviors increased after the reform, and the number of pupils in special education more than doubled between school years 1974–75 and 1979–80 (Somerkivi 1982, 40).

Finally, one of the main reasons to favor the delay in tracking is that the likelihood of a student being placed in the “wrong” track is reduced and the anxiety associated with tracking lessens, since the amount of pretracking information about students’ abilities is higher at the time of the tracking decision (Brunello, Giannini, and Ariga 2007). This suggests that the reform improves mental health. However, Pekkarinen (2008) argues that for boys, this benefit is offset by the fact that tracking now occurs during puberty, in contrast to girls, for whom it occurs after puberty.

In short, previous research does not clearly predict the reform’s effect on mental health, since different mechanisms are pushing in different directions. However, it does suggest that the effect likely differs by gender and academic ability.

III.  Data

To evaluate the long-run effect of the reform on mental health, we link three data sets: (1) census data covering the total population of Finland, (2) data on the causes of death from the comprehensive death certificates, and (3) complete hospital admissions data.

A.  Census Data

We use the population register data of permanent residents of Finland. The data originate from the Longitudinal Population Census Files from Statistics Finland. Demographic and labor market information are available for the years 1975 and 1985 and after that annually over the period 1987–2014. The municipality of residence is recorded annually since 1971. The data contain almost complete household and parental links. The date and municipality of birth are also recorded. Furthermore, the data also contain detailed information about degrees completed from 1970 onward.

The core data include the individuals born in Finland between 1962 and 1966, following Pekkala Kerr, Pekkarinen, and Uusitalo (2013, 586). We start with birth cohort 1962 and end with birth cohort 1966 to increase the homogeneity of the cohorts under study.8 This sample restriction also allows us to follow all birth cohorts up to the maximum age of 45.9 As shown in table 1, there are between 72,248 and 74,248 persons in each birth cohort. Hence, we have approximately 366,000 individuals in total. Annual information about the municipality of residence, together with the birth date, determines whether a pupil attended the tracked or comprehensive school system. The reform was effective for students who were at most 11 years old, that is, entering fifth grade, at the end of the year in which the reform was implemented in their region of residence. For instance, people who turned 11 in the region in which the reform was implemented in 1975 received postreform schooling if they were born in 1964, 1965, or 1966 and prereform schooling if they were born in 1962 or 1963.

Table 1. 

Number of Observations and Adoption of the Reform

Birth CohortAdoption Year in the Municipality 
197219731974197519761977Total
19627,46010,73914,36114,96515,4659,34872,338
19637,40210,65615,50715,15715,9349,59274,248
19647,11210,30915,08615,71016,0139,86574,095
19656,6469,75414,60815,54816,3159,91372,784
19666,5249,63814,42515,14616,72610,39072,849
Total35,14451,09673,98776,52680,45349,108366,314

Note.  For each birth cohort, people affected by the reform were those who lived in municipalities where the reform was adopted in the years in boldface font. For instance, for people born in 1962, the treatment group consists of people who at age 11, i.e., in 1973, lived in a municipality in which the reform was implemented in 1972 or 1973.

View Table Image

We exclude foreign-born individuals (most of whom immigrated to Finland after the reform) and those living in the Åland Islands (in total, 8% of the original sample) from the estimation sample, retaining individuals living in 465 different municipalities. We also exclude a small number of individuals who, at ages 11–15, migrated between municipalities with a different year of adoption of the reform (less than 2.6% of the original sample),10 since the reform indicator cannot be assigned unambiguously for these people. Finally, we exclude a very small number of emigrants from the original data.

Since measures of academic ability before tracking are not available in the data, we investigate heterogeneity by parents’ education.11 The variable is categorized into three values: (1) low educated (53%), if neither of the parents completed postcompulsory education, that is, if they both had a maximum of nine years of schooling; (2) mid-educated (27%), if either or both of the parents completed a vocational degree but neither studied further; and (3) highly educated (20%), if at least one of them completed high school or a higher level of (tertiary) education (see fig. 1 for a reminder of the prereform system).

B.  Mortality and Mental Health Disorders

To relate our findings to previous research on the education-health nexus (Lleras-Muney 2005; Clark and Royer 2013; Meghir, Palme, and Simeonova 2018), we examine mortality outcomes. We use data regarding the year and cause of death from the comprehensive death certificates (until 2013). All diagnoses of the causes of death pass a routine validation conducted by Statistics Finland, and unclear cases are judged by a panel (Lahti and Penttilä 2001). Our mortality outcomes (suicides, mental health–related deaths, and all-cause mortality) are measured until age of 45. We picked age 45 so that all birth cohorts could be followed over the same window.

We use suicides as the primary mortality variable (a dummy for occurrence before age 45). Suicides are defined by the codes X60–X84 and Y87.0 in the International Classification of Diseases, 10th revision (ICD-10), which is the standard diagnostic tool for clinical purposes. Suicide is a relevant outcome for three reasons. First, suicides are closely related to mental health problems. For example, approximately 90% of suicides are associated with psychiatric disorders (Henriksson et al. 1993; Pirkola et al. 2009). Second, approximately 25% of all deaths by age 45 are suicides. Third, the suicide mortality of young Finns is among the highest in the world (Lahti et al. 2011).

Following Alexander and Schnell (2019), we also consider a broader measure of mental health–related deaths, which includes not only suicides but also injuries of undetermined intent (i.e., fatal injuries about which it is not known whether they occurred accidentally or were purposely inflicted) and accidental deaths involving poisonings, drownings, and deaths involving firearms and trains. In our data, 69.3% of mental health–related deaths by age 45 are suicides, 3.6% are injuries of undermined intent, 19.8% are accidental poisonings,12 5.7% are accidental drownings, and 1.5% are accidents involving firearms or trains. ICD codes that are used to identify these causes of death are given in appendix A. We utilize this broader measure of mental health–related mortality because not necessarily all deaths caused by mental health disorders are classified as suicides. Finally, all-cause mortality by age 45 is measured by a dummy variable (table 2). In our data, 4.6% of men and 1.8% of women die by age 45. Moreover, the mental health–related death rate for men is higher (1.9%) than that for women (1.2%).

Table 2. 

Summary Statistics, by Gender

VariableMalesFemales
MeanStandard DeviationMeanStandard Deviation
Has a high school degree.291.454.498.500
Years of schooling12.7772.72313.4892.635
Mortality:    
 Suicide by age 45.013.113.003.055
 Mental health–related death by age 45.019.136.012.107
 Death by age 45.046.209.018.131
Hospitalizations due to mental health disorder:  
 At ages 6–10.006.080.004.063
 At ages 11–15.006.079.005.070
 At ages 16–25.044.205.017.127
 At ages 26–35.044.206.028.166
 At ages 36–45.050.219.034.181
 At ages 16–45.100.300.056.231
Hospitalizations at ages 16–45 due to:  
 Schizophrenia.012.109.008.092
 Other nonaffective psychosis.018.132.015.123
 Bipolar disorder.005.070.005.069
 Depressive disorder.017.131.019.136
 Anxiety, stress, neurotic disorder.016.127.007.084
 Substance-use disorder.042.200.013.115
Mental health–related hospitalization days at ages 16–45:
 Unconditional15.017172.3910.861133.25
 Conditional on being hospitalized150.66527.00192.59529.01
Parental education:    
 Low educated.530.499.529.499
 Mid-educated.266.442.268.443
 Highly educated.204.403.203.402
Observations186,777179,537

Note.  “Low-educated” parents: neither parent completed postcompulsory schooling; “mid-educated” parents: at least one parent completed a vocational degree but not more; “highly educated” parents: at least one parent completed a higher education degree (including high school). The sum of the last two education dummies is equal to the parental education variable used in Pekkala Kerr, Pekkarinen, and Uusitalo (2013).

View Table Image

We then study the effects of the reform on mental health–related hospitalizations, using inpatient data.13 We focus on serious mental health–related hospitalizations for two reasons. First, their treatment costs are particularly high in the universal health care system. Second, severe mental illnesses cause substantial indirect costs in terms of absenteeism, weak long-run labor market attachment, and early disability pensions (Hakulinen et al. 2019). Therefore, undoubtedly, society cares about these outcomes.

We use register-based measures that are free from the potential measurement error inherent to self-reported mental health symptoms (Ritter et al. 2001). Information about mental health disorders is extracted from the Hospital Discharge Register (HDR) compiled by the National Institute for Health and Welfare for the period 1969–2013. The data include dates of admission to the hospital, dates of discharge, and the primary reason for hospitalization. Diagnosis codes are from the eighth, ninth, and 10th revisions of the ICD. Spells due to mental health disorders correspond to a diagnosis code starting with the letter F in the ICD-10 classification and to codes 290–319 in ICD-8 or ICD-9. Validation studies have shown that the HDR data are of high quality from 1972 onward (Sund 2012).

In the baseline model, we use a dummy variable to indicate whether the individual had any mental health–related hospitalizations between ages 16 and 45.14 According to the data, there are 138,800 mental health–related hospitalization spells between ages 16 and 45 among 28,700 individuals (representing 7.8% of the total population). Approximately 10% of men and 6% of women had mental health problems that resulted in hospitalization (table 2). Conditional on having a mental health–related hospitalization, the average time spent in the hospital between ages 16 and 45 is 151 days for men and 193 days for women.15 We then also consider separately whether the individual had any spell starting at ages 11–15, 16–25, 26–35, or 36–45. Again, we record new hospitalization spells until age 45 so that all birth cohorts can be followed during the same age window.

To get a comprehensive picture, we also examine the effect of the reform on different types of mental health disorders (Suvisaari et al. 2009; Santavirta et al. 2015): (1) schizophrenia, a mental disorder characterized by hallucinations, delusions, and cognitive deficits; (2) other psychoses that are not related to emotions or moods (nonaffective psychosis); (3) bipolar disorder, an affective psychosis involving emotional and mood abnormalities (and manic episodes); (4) depressive disorder, which can include repeated episodes of severe depression or chronic mild-grade depression (dysthymia); (5) severe anxiety, stress, and neurotic disorders, which can interfere with daily activities, such as job performance, school work, and social relationships; and (6) substance-use disorder, which includes all psychiatric hospitalizations related to alcohol or substance abuse or addiction. Appendix A contains details about the codes used to define these categories.

Table 2 provides the summary statistics for the main outcomes of interest, broken down by gender, and table B1 provides these same statistics, broken down by socioeconomic background, in addition to some summary statistics on additional outcomes.

IV.  Empirical Approach

To identify the average long-run effects of the comprehensive school reform, we estimate difference-in-differences models with the following structure:

(1)yijc=α+ηj+τc+β·REFORMjc+γXi+ϵijc,
where yijc is the health outcome of individual i, who was born in year c and schooled in municipality j when entering fifth grade; ηj and τc are the municipality and the birth cohort fixed effects.16 There are permanent regional differences in the outcomes that we need to control for. Similarly, birth cohorts may have also been exposed to different shocks in childhood and adolescence that have an impact on mental health in adulthood. The term REFORMjc is a dummy that varies across municipalities and cohorts and equals one if individual i has been exposed to the reform, that is, has experienced comprehensive school until age 16. Thus, β is the policy parameter of interest in the models. The baseline specification does not include control variables Xi except a constant. We check the sensitivity of our results to the inclusion of controls.

Since we identify the estimates using a difference-in-differences framework, the timing of when municipalities adopt the reform needs to be unrelated to differences in cohort trends in mental health disorders across municipalities. Figure B1 provides evidence for the lack of relationship between all six types of individual mental health disorders studied and the timing of the reform (following Bhuller, Mogstad, and Salvanes 2017). We also show that there is no evidence of a systematic relationship between the timing of the reform and baseline pretreatment municipality characteristics that may affect mental health disorders (fig. B2).

The estimated models identify average treatment effects for the treated. We report estimates from linear probability models, since they facilitate the interpretation of the estimated coefficients and are less sensitive to distributional assumptions (Wooldridge 2001).

Following Meghir, Palme, and Simeonova (2018) and insights from Galama, Lleras-Muney, and van Kippersluis (2018), we separately estimate the empirical specifications by gender, since there is substantial variation in all outcomes by gender. For example, suicide mortality is much higher among males. We also estimate the models by level of parental education (three mutually exclusive categories), since earlier research suggests that the effects of the reform may differ significantly by socioeconomic background (Pekkala Kerr, Pekkarinen, and Uusitalo 2013). Additionally, there may be socioeconomic differences in the utilization of hospital care, even though Finland has a universal health care system (Gerdtham 1997; Bijwaard, Myrskylä, and Tyneliu 2018). Possible regional differences in the utilization of hospital care are captured by the municipality fixed effects that are included in all models.

Throughout the paper, standard errors are clustered at the municipal level, which is the level of policy variation. We also report alternative significance levels of the main results, using clustering at the larger NUTS-4 level (67 regions; table B13). In addition, we report significance levels based on adjusted standard errors that account for testing multiple hypotheses. We apply the step-down approach of Romano and Wolf (2005), which takes advantage of the dependence structure of individual tests.17

We provide robustness checks for our baseline results. First, we control for region-specific linear time trends, which makes identification of the effects less reliant on the common-trend assumption. Second, we use mental health–related hospitalizations at ages 6–10 as an additional control to account for the possible relationship between prior mental health disorders and treatment status. Third, we estimate models with and without the Helsinki metropolitan area, since some private schools were operating in the Helsinki region after the reform (Pekkarinen, Uusitalo, and Pekkala Kerr 2009). Fourth, we reestimate the baseline models using a subsample of individuals who were 11 years old at most 3 years before or at most 3 years after the adoption of the reform (in their municipality of residence). This restriction was imposed so as to rely only on individuals who participated in compulsory schooling closest to the adoption of the reform (i.e., diagonal elements in table 1); this increases the homogeneity of the treated and untreated birth cohorts (see also table B6). Fifth, we augment the baseline model by expanding the sample to also include individuals born in 1960–61, as in Pekkarinen, Uusitalo, and Pekkala Kerr (2009). In comparison, our preferred sample focuses on individuals born in 1962–66, the same years as in Pekkala Kerr, Pekkarinen, and Uusitalo (2013). These robustness checks are reported in appendix B for suicides (table B7), mental health–related deaths (table B8), all-cause mortality (table B9), and mental health–related hospitalizations (table B10), and brief comments regarding them are given in section V.18

V.  Results

A.  Graphical Presentation of the Main Results

To check the validity of our research design, as well as to graphically preview the key findings, we estimated the baseline specification using lead and lag year dummies around the reform and omitting the year before the reform (t=−1), as in Pekkala Kerr, Pekkarinen, and Uusitalo (2013):

(2)yijct=α+ηj+τc+k1βk·1[k=t]+ϵijct,
where 1 is an indicator function and βk are the parameters of the event time dummies. Time t=0 represents the first birth cohort in the municipality affected by the reform (table B6). This model allows us to separate the preexisting trends from policy responses over time.

The results reported in figures 3, 4, B3, and B4 suggest that our main findings are not affected by prereform trends, which provides evidence in favor of our identification assumption.19 They also preview that our main findings are going to be mostly null effects. We now turn to regressions where we pool together all the postreform (and prereform) years to precisely estimate the magnitude of these (null) effects.

Figure 3. 
Figure 3. 

Effect of the reform on suicide at ages 16–45: leads and lags around the year before the reform. Plots are based on baseline regression models, where the reform dummy is replaced with year dummies for the leads and lags around the reform year. The plotted points represent the estimates on the lead and lag dummies. The omitted category is the year before the reform (−1). The estimated effects are reported, together with their 95% confidence intervals (based on robust standard errors clustered at the municipal level).

Figure 4. 
Figure 4. 

Effect of the reform on mental health–related hospitalizations at ages 16–45: leads and lags around the year before the reform. Plots are based on baseline regression models, where the reform dummy is replaced with year dummies for the leads and lags around the reform year. The plotted points represent the estimates on the lead and lag dummies. The omitted category is the year before the reform (−1). The estimated effects are reported, together with their 95% confidence intervals (based on robust standard errors clustered at the municipal level).

B.  Mortality

Table 3 reports the estimates for the effects of the reform on the incidence of suicide by age 45. Regardless of the sample, we find no evidence that the reform affected suicides by age 45. For instance, for males, we can rule out increases of 0.14 percentage points or decreases of 0.18 percentage points, relative to the mean outcome of 1.3%. Table B7 shows that the results for the full sample are robust to the variations of the model (e.g., adding controls and excluding some observations).20

Table 3. 

Effect of the Reform on Suicide by Age 45

 Full SampleParental Education
LowMidHigh
 Males
Treatment effect−.0002−.0016.0013.0003
 (.0008)(.0011)(.0010)(.0010)
R2.0032.0054.0077.0132
Mean outcome.0129.0099.0034.0023
Observations186,77794,03747,22436,282
 Females
Treatment effect−.0003−.0006−.0001.0002
 (.0005)(.0005)(.0004)(.0007)
R2.0025.0044.0095.0148
Mean outcome.0030.0021.0007.0009
Observations179,53790,88146,09234,828

Note.  The full sample also includes individuals for whom information about parents (such as parental education) is missing. All models include cohort and municipality fixed effects. Standard errors, reported in parentheses, are clustered at the municipal level. None of the coefficients are significant at the 10% level (two-sided tests).

View Table Image

Next, we consider the effects of the reform on mental health–related deaths, which include suicides, injuries of undetermined intent, and accidental deaths. The results, reported in table B2, are similar to those found for suicides. The only notable exception is that for this broader measure of mental health–related deaths, improved survival in males from low-educated families can be seen. The estimate indicates reduction in mortality by 0.3 percentage points (significant at the 5% level), relative to the mean outcome of 1.4%. However, after adjustment for multiple-hypothesis testing, its significance level is marginally outside the 10% level (table B11). The results for males from low-educated families remain qualitatively intact to the alternative specifications of the model, but the significance levels vary by model (table B8). The estimated effects remain insignificant in other subsamples.

To evaluate the overall health effects of the reform, table B3 reports the findings on all-cause mortality. Echoing the results for suicides, we find no evidence that the reform affected all-cause mortality by age 45. The estimated effects are close to zero.21 For males, we can rule out increases of 0.17 percentage points or decreases of 0.5 percentage points at the 5% significance level, relative to the mean outcome of 4.6%. For females, we can rule out increases of 0.4 percentage points or decreases of 0.15 percentage points, relative to the mean outcome of 1.8%. Despite the universal health care system, the baseline differences in mortality by parental education are substantial—males from low-educated families have a three times higher mortality than those from highly educated families—but the treatment effect is insignificant across groups. Robustness of these results is documented in table B9.

The conclusions are also not sensitive to the use of a Cox proportional hazards model (table B16). These results provide opportunity to compare the precision of our estimates to prior evidence. Meghir, Palme, and Simeonova (2018) report the effects of the reform on hazard rates of death by age 45 in their appendix table A5 (sample of males and females). Their estimate from a Cox regression is 1.0006 (SE of 0.0217). We report a comparable estimate in the note to table B16, which indicates a combined reform effect of 0.985 (SE of 0.030). Thus, Meghir, Palme, and Simeonova (2018) can rule out (with 95% confidence) changes in the risk of death by age 45 that are outside −4.2% and +4.3%, whereas we can rule out changes that are outside −7.3% and +4.6%. In both studies, standard errors are clustered at the municipal level.

C.  Hospitalization

Table 4 reports the results that use the incidence of mental health–related hospitalizations as an outcome variable over the age range 16–45. The average of the outcome variable is 10% and 6% for males and females, respectively. These figures indicate that these incidents are not rare in our data.

Table 4. 

Effect of the Reform on Mental Health–Related Hospitalizations at Ages 16–45

 Full SampleParental Education
LowMidHigh
 Males
Treatment effect.0027.0074**.0012−.0058
 (.0025)(.0036)(.0045)(.0043)
R2.0054.0092.0136.0128
Mean outcome.0997.1080.0847.0735
Observations186,77794,03747,22436,282
 Females
Treatment effect.0029.0022−.0007.0095**
 (.0019)(.0025)(.0034)(.0041)
R2.0042.0071.0106.0133
Mean outcome.0564.0593.0498.0521
Observations179,53790,88146,09234,828

Note.  The full sample also includes individuals for whom information about parents (such as parental education) is missing. All models include cohort and municipality fixed effects. Standard errors reported in parenthesis are clustered at the municipal level. Coefficients in italics survive a Romano-Wolf correction for multiple-hypothesis testing (Romano and Wolf 2005) at the 10% significance level (see table B11).

** Significant at the 5% level (all two-sided tests).

View Table Image

The reform led to an increase in mental health–related hospitalizations, but for only some subgroups. The first finding is that males from low-educated families who were exposed to late tracking have a 0.74 percentage points higher probability of having severe mental health disorders that result in hospitalization than those who were educated in the prereform system, that is, engaged in early tracking. The size of the effect represents a 7% increase relative to a mean outcome of 10.8%. The effect is robust to using an outcome measure that captures also the intensive margin and not just the extensive one, that is, the number of days spent in the hospital for mental health reasons between ages 16 and 45 (see table B17). Importantly, the effect for males from low-educated families is no longer statistically significant after the standard errors are adjusted for a Romano-Wolf correction for multiple-hypothesis testing (Romano and Wolf 2005).

The second finding is that, because of the postponement of the tracking age from 11 to age 16, for females from highly educated families the probability of severe mental health disorders increased by 0.95 percentage points. We highlight this finding for several reasons. First, the quantitative magnitude of the effect is rather large, given the low baseline probability of mental health disorders for women from highly educated families (5.2%). Second, the effect remains significant (at 10% level) even after the standard errors are adjusted for Romano-Wolf correction for multiple-hypothesis testing (Romano and Wolf 2005; table B11). Third, the effect remains intact regardless of whether we control for the incidence of prior mental health disorders and/or parents’ mental health status during the prereform period (table B10). Fourth, the finding is also robust to several other sensitivity checks, for example, accounting for the full set of region-specific linear time trends (table B10). Fifth, the estimated dynamic response models show that the finding is not driven by prereform trends (fig. 4). Therefore, it is unlikely that the result is affected by unobserved regional characteristics that are potentially correlated with the rollout of the reform. Sixth, the finding remains intact even if individuals are classified into treatment on the basis of their municipality of birth (table B18) or municipality at age 10 (table B19) instead of municipality at age 11 (and not eliminating individuals who migrate between ages of 11 and 16).

The historical timing of the reform, together with the availability of longitudinal data, enables us to examine the effects of the reform over the life cycle. We report the results for relevant age categories in table 5. We find that at ages 36–45, for females from high-educated families, mental health disorders are significantly more likely to occur in those who had been exposed to the postreform school system than in those who were educated in the prereform system. However, for this affected group, we do not find significant effects during the school years. Additionally, for males from low-educated families, the positive effect of the comprehensive school reform on the probability of hospitalization for mental health reasons peaks at ages 26–35, but there is also a significant effect at later ages, that is, between the ages of 36 and 45.

Table 5. 

Effect of the Reform on Mental Health–Related Hospitalizations over the Life Cycle

 Full SampleParental Education
LowMidHigh
 Males
Ages 11–15−.0002.0011−.0016−.0016
 (.0008)(.0009)(.0012)(.0013)
Ages 16–25.0024.0041−.0007.0027
 (.0021)(.0031)(.0040)(.0028)
Ages 26–35.0020.0062***.0003−.0057
 (.0016)(.0022)(.0031)(.0035)
Ages 36–45.0018.0046*−.0018−.0032
 (.0016)(.0023)(.0031)(.0032)
Observations186,77794,03747,22436,282
 Females
Ages 11–15−.0002−.0009.0024**−.0017
 (.0006)(.0008)(.0010)(.0014)
Ages 16–25.0005.0007−.0012.0009
 (.0011)(.0015)(.0021)(.0023)
Ages 26–35.0020.0011.0034.0026
 (.0012)(.0016)(.0024)(.0029)
Ages 36–45.0012.0018−.0040.0058**
 (.0014)(.0021)(.0025)(.0027)
Observations179,53790,88146,09234,828

Note.  The table reports the treatment effect of the reform. Each row corresponds to a different outcome, and each column corresponds to a different sample. See table B1 for the mean values of the outcome variables by parental education and figure B5 for a graphical illustration. The full sample also includes individuals for whom information about parents (such as parental education) is missing. Individuals who died before the observation period were removed from the estimation sample. All models include cohort and municipality fixed effects. Standard errors, reported in parentheses, are clustered at the municipal level. Coefficients in italics survive a Romano-Wolf correction for multiple-hypothesis testing (Romano and Wolf 2005) at the 10% significance level (see table B11).

* Significant at the 10% level (all two-sided tests).

** Significant at the 5% level (all two-sided tests).

*** Significant at the 1% level (all two-sided tests).

View Table Image

Table 6 examines whether any specific disorder drives the estimated treatment effects. We observe a significant increase in the probability of depressive disorders for females from highly educated families. Conversely, for males from low-educated families, the overall increase in hospitalizations is driven by the increase in alcohol-related mental disorders. Alcohol abuse may be caused by self-medication related to perceived stress (Enoch 2011). The gender pattern that we observe is plausible, since in our data, alcohol-related mental disorders and depressive disorders are the most prevalent mental health problems for males and females, respectively (table 2).

Table 6. 

Effect of the Reform on Specific Mental Health–Related Hospitalizations at Ages 16–45

  Parental Education
Full SampleLowMidHigh
 Males
Schizophrenia.0005.0018−.0013.0014
 (.0008)(.0013)(.0017)(.0018)
Other nonaffective psychosis−.0005.0013−.0007−.0028
 (.0009)(.0015)(.0022)(.0020)
Bipolar disorder.0007.0011.0015−.0012
 (.0005)(.0008)(.0010)(.0013)
Depressive disorder.0013.0017.0007−.0033
 (.0010)(.0015)(.0019)(.0021)
Anxiety, stress, neurotic disorder.0010.0026−.0015−.0013
 (.0013)(.0018)(.0021)(.0018)
Substance-use disorder.0038**.0048**.0015.0021
 (.0016)(.0023)(.0030)(.0025)
Observations186,77794,03747,22436,282
 Females
Schizophrenia.0004.0013.0001−.0017
 (.0008)(.0010)(.0013)(.0020)
Other nonaffective psychosis.0006.0015−.0016−.0000
 (.0010)(.0013)(.0020)(.0021)
Bipolar disorder.0005−.0000−.0007.0026*
 (.0005)(.0007)(.0011)(.0014)
Depressive disorder.0015.0009.0004.0062***
 (.0009)(.0015)(.0020)(.0022)
Anxiety, stress, neurotic disorder.0001.0003.0006−.0010
 (.0008)(.0011)(.0012)(.0016)
Substance-use disorder.0009.0021−.0013.0024
 (.0009)(.0014)(.0018)(.0015)
Observations179,53790,88146,09234,828

Note.  The table reports the treatment effect of the reform on various outcomes. Each row corresponds to a different outcome, and each column corresponds to a different sample. See table B1 for the mean values of the outcome variables by parental education. The full sample also includes individuals for whom information about parents (such as parental education) is missing. All models include cohort and municipality fixed effects. Standard errors, reported in parentheses, are clustered at the municipal level. Coefficients in italics survive a Romano-Wolf correction for multiple-hypothesis testing (Romano and Wolf 2005) at the 10% significance level (see table B11).

* Significant at the 10% level (all two-sided tests).

** Significant at the 5% level (all two-sided tests).

*** Significant at the 1% level (all two-sided tests).

View Table Image

VI.  Potential Mechanisms

The reform could have affected mental health through various channels discussed in section II. On the positive side, in addition to exposure to higher-achieving peers and access to a larger set of cognitive skills for students who would have started vocational training at age 11 without the reform, the key theoretical argument in favor of postponing the tracking is that it would have allowed the system to have more relevant and accurate information about abilities and comparative advantages when matching students to a particular education (Brunello, Giannini, and Ariga 2007). Thus, increasing the tracking age should improve the efficiency of the match and subsequent labor market outcomes, which could translate into better mental health later in adulthood. By contrast, we do not find any improvement in mental health as a result of the comprehensive school reform.

In fact, we find that the reform did not lead to an improvement in education or labor market outcomes (tables B20, B21). For students from low-educated families, we even find some negative effects of the reform on educational achievements and economic outcomes. These results suggest that the theoretical mechanisms related to the efficiency of matching students to suitable education are not at play here.

To further pin down the potential mechanisms, we follow Acharya, Blackwell, and Sen (2016) and estimate the average controlled direct effects of the reform, as described in appendix C. The negative effects on mental health of females from highly educated families remain intact even when the education and income mediators are controlled for, as can be seen in table 7. Thus, for females, the observed effects on mental health cannot be explained by the education/income channels.

Table 7. 

Estimated Reform Effect, Controlling for Education and Income Mediators Later in Life

OutcomeBaseline EstimatesMediation Analysis
EducationEducation and Income
 A. Males, Low-Educated Parents
Suicide by age 45−.0016−.0018−.0017*
 (.0011)(.0012)(.0010)
Mental health–related death by age 45−.0030**−.0033**−.0027**
 (.0014)(.0014)(.0013)
Death by age 45−.0034−.0040*−.0042**
 (.0022)(.0022)(.0021)
MHD at ages 16–45.0074**.0061*.0060
 (.0036)(.0036)(.0037)
MHD at ages 26–35.0062***.0056**.0049*
 (.0022)(.0022)(.0022)
MHD at ages 36–45.0046*.0038.0032
 (.0023)(.0024)(.0024)
Substance-use disorder at ages 16–45.0048**.0041*.0033
 (.0023)(.0024)(.0023)
 B. Females, Highly Educated Parents
Suicide by age 45.0002.0003.0005
 (.0007)(.0007)(.0007)
Mental health–related death by age 45.0005.0005.0007
 (.0008)(.0009)(.0008)
Death by age 45−.0008−.0008−.0008
 (.0012)(.0013)(.0012)
MHD at ages 16–45.0095**.0096**.0097**
 (.0041)(.0041)(.0042)
MHD at ages 26–35.0026.0026.0025
 (.0029)(.0029)(.0029)
MHD at ages 36–45.0058**.0057**.0061**
 (.0027)(.0028)(.0029)
Depression at ages 16–45.0062***.0063***.0065***
 (.0022)(.0022)(.0022)

Note.  We report the results only on samples (outcomes) that show significant effects in tables 36, B2, and B3. See table B23 for corresponding results for a full sample of males or females. MHD = Mental health disorder requiring hospitalization. Columns 2 and 3 present controlled direct effects based on Acharya, Blackwell, and Sen (2016). Education controls include the years of schooling and a dummy for having a high school degree. Income control is the log of taxable income at ages 26–35. Each cell reports the estimated effect of the comprehensive school reform from separate models. All models include cohort and municipality fixed effects. Standard errors reported in parenthesis are clustered at the municipal level. In cols. 2 and 3, standard errors have been bootstrapped using 1,000 replications and clustered at the municipal level. Coefficients in italics survive a Romano-Wolf correction for multiple-hypothesis testing (Romano and Wolf 2005) at the 10% significance level (see table B22 for more details).

* Significant at the 10% level (all two-sided tests).

** Significant at the 5% level (all two-sided tests).

*** Significant at the 1% level (all two-sided tests).

View Table Image

Instead, we conjecture that peer effects constitute a potential mechanism driving the observed adverse effects for females from highly educated families. These females, before the reform, would most likely have been tracked into the selective academic curriculum from age 11 to 16 and exposed only to high-ability peers. However, after the reform, they are now exposed to comprehensive school and to peers who would have pursued the vocational track without the reform. Presumably, this new set of peers, on average, is often less well-behaved and of lower academic ability. This may result in a higher probability of being exposed to disruptive behaviors. Prior work has shown the negative association between being bullied at school and mental health in adulthood (Sigurdson, Wallander, and Sund 2014). In particular, bullied girls, rather than bullied boys, even infrequently bullied ones, are more likely to suffer from depression symptoms in adulthood (Brunstein Klomek et al. 2007, 43). Further empirical research is needed to establish whether change in the exposure to disruptive behaviors is driving our results.

An alternative interpretation for the observed adverse effects relies on the more intensive competition induced by the reform. Indeed, the reform implied that children from disadvantaged backgrounds now had a better chance to compete for places in the higher education system: it equalized opportunities by socioeconomic status and decreased the intergenerational correlation of earnings (Pekkarinen, Uusitalo, and Pekkala Kerr 2009). After the reform, children from privileged backgrounds faced more effective competition from the most talented children from disadvantaged backgrounds. This change may have affected females more, as females tend to respond less favorably to more intense competition than males (Niederle and Vesterlund 2011).

In summary, we do not find evidence supporting a role for the education/income channels (see also table B23). Since the reform had a slightly negative effect on income in adulthood, this could push toward lower mental health, and it could be that, once we control for income, the reform had a positive effect on mental health. However, since our earlier results remain intact after we control for education/income, we interpret these results as further evidence that the reform, overall, had no effect on mental health rather than multiple effects of different signs cancelling each other out.

VII.  Conclusion

We contribute to the limited literature on the effects of school tracking regimes in Europe. Our results are based on a comprehensive school reform that was rolled out gradually across Finnish municipalities during 1972–77. The reform resulted in children from different socioeconomic backgrounds and potentially different academic abilities being held in the same classes for five extra years. The reform also changed the type of education received, providing five additional years of general education to students before the start of tracking. Consequently, our study also contributes to a growing literature illustrating the importance of human capital measures other than simply the quantity of education. In their review, Galama, Lleras-Muney, and van Kippersluis (2018) highlight that for health-related behaviors, the type of education received—general, vocational, or academic—is more important than the length of education. As outcome variables, we focus on severe mental disorders, which cause substantial costs to the health care system and lead to lasting negative outcomes at the individual level, such as poor labor market attachment (Ettner, Frank, and Kessler 1997).

Although the generalization of our estimates to current policy settings is not straightforward, the long-run health effects of school reforms can be identified only for birth cohorts treated many decades ago. Furthermore, we believe that our results can provide potentially valuable insights for other types of school tracking systems, such as those that identify “gifted” or “special education” students early during schooling and provide either additional opportunities or segregated instruction alongside similar students.

Overall, we find no significant effects on mental health–related hospitalizations or deaths, even though the average zero effects are precisely estimated. However, since we use register data on hospitalizations and deaths, our results do not rule out the possible effects of the reform on less serious mental health disorders. Heterogeneity analysis shows that postponing the age of tracking had an adverse long-run effect on mental health outcomes for females from highly educated families, as they were more likely to be hospitalized for depression after the reform. Thus, increasing the age of tracking may come at a cost of negative mental health effects for some groups. Furthermore, we find that the result for females is not accounted for by changes in education or income induced by the reform. Instead, we propose that peer effects can possibly explain our finding regarding the affected females. Exploring this mechanism is a promising avenue for future research.

Appendix A.  ICD-8, ICD-9, and ICD-10 Diagnosis Codes

A1.  Mental Health–Related Deaths

1.  Suicides

a.  ICD-8, ICD-9: E950–E959;

b.  ICD-10: X60–X84, Y87.0.

2.  Injuries of undetermined intent (i.e., undetermined whether accidentally or purposely inflicted)

a.  ICD-8, ICD-9: E980–E989;

b.  ICD-10: Y10–Y34, Y87.2, Y89.9.

3.  Accidental deaths

a.  Poisoning (i.e., accidental poisoning and exposure to noxious substances)

i.  ICD-8: E850–E877;

ii.  ICD-9: E850–E869;

iii.  ICD-10: X40–X49.

b.  Accidental drowning and submersion

i.  ICD-8, ICD-9: E910;

ii.  ICD-10: W65–W74.

c.  Firearms (i.e., accidental discharge of firearms)

i.  ICD-8, ICD-9: E922;

ii.  ICD-10: W32–W34.

d.  Trains (i.e., railway accidents and motor vehicle accident involving collision with train)

i.  ICD-8, ICD-9: E800–E807, E810;

ii.  ICD-10: V05, V15, V80.6, V81.2–V81.9; V25, V35, V45, V55, V65, V75, V81.0, V81.1, V87.6, V88.6.

A2.  Mental Health–Related Hospitalizations

1.  Schizophrenia

a.  ICD-8, ICD-9: 295.0–295.3, 295.5, 295.6, 295.8, 295.9;

b.  ICD-10: F20.

2.  Other nonaffective psychosis

a.  ICD-8, ICD-9: 295.4, 295.7, 297, 298, 299;

b.  ICD-10: F22–F25, F28, F29.

3.  Bipolar disorder

a.  ICD-8: 2961, 2963;

b.  ICD-9: 2962–2967;

c.  ICD-10: F30–F31.

4.  Depressive disorder

a.  ICD-8: 2960, 2962;

b.  ICD-9: 2961, 3004A;

c.  ICD-10: F32–F33, F341.

5.  Anxiety, stress, neurotic disorder

a.  ICD-8, ICD-9: 3000, 3002, 3003;

b.  ICD-10: F40–F42, F430–F431.

6.  Substance-use disorder

a.  ICD-8: 291, 303–304;

b.  ICD-9: 291–292, 303–305;

c.  ICD-10: F10–F19.

Appendix B.  Tables and Figures

Table B1. 

Mean Values by Sex and Parental Background

VariableMalesFemales
Low-Educated ParentsMid-Educated ParentsHighly Educated ParentsLow-Educated ParentsMid-Educated ParentsHighly Educated Parents
Mortality:      
 Suicide by age 45.0099.0034.0023.0020.0007.0009
 Mental health–related death by age 45.0143.0051.0036.0031.0012.0012
 Death by age 45 (all-cause mortality).0339.0132.0102.0122.0050.0047
Hospitalization due to mental health disorder:
 At ages 6–10.0066.0061.0046.0043.0038.0027
 At ages 11–15.0068.0057.0034.0052.0041.0041
 At ages 16–25.047.038.032.017.014.016
 At ages 26–35.047.037.032.029.025.027
 At ages 36–45.056.042.036.036.031.030
 At ages 16–45.108.085.074.059.050.052
Hospitalizations at ages 16–45 due to:
 Schizophrenia.012.010.012.008.008.009
 Other nonaffective psychosis.018.015.016.015.014.016
 Bipolar disorder.005.005.005.005.004.006
 Depressive disorder.019.015.014.020.017.018
 Anxiety, stress, neurotic disorder.017.014.013.007.006.006
 Substance-use disorder.047.033.023.015.010.009
Mental health–related hospitalization days at ages 16–45:
 Unconditional15.47412.47713.70910.9469.25911.784
 Conditional on being hospitalized143.63147.29186.49184.60186.01226.15
Educational outcomes:
 Years of schooling12.18312.79014.57712.97813.53214.977
 Has a high school (HS) degree.177.265.636.383.505.790
 HS exam score in native language (percentile).431.429.503.457.472.568
 HS exam score in advanced math (percentile).500.511.571.447.455.528
 HS exam score in basic math (percentile).485.493.535.472.487.556
 Has vocational secondary education (highest).536.502.232.454.394.194
 Has completed vocational college degree (highest).116.157.192.244.275.264
 Has completed university master’s degree (highest).055.090.301.086.131.337
Labor market outcomes:
 Average income at ages 26–45 (€000s)29.26932.16441.65722.07323.74528.691
 Average income at ages 26–35 (€000s)24.12425.76230.46018.43419.44622.480
 Average income at ages 36–45 (€000s)34.62238.65255.01825.77628.06835.012
 Employment rate at ages 26–45.765.806.815.732.760.764
 Employment rate at ages 26–35.739.775.776.668.701.704
 Employment rate at ages 36–45.799.841.858.790.821.824
Parental characteristics:
 Parents’ average taxable income in 1975 and 198515.71618.63931.13715.71118.58630.942
No. of individuals94,03747,22436,28290,88146,09234,828

Note.  All income figures are deflated to 2012.

View Table Image: 1 | 2
Figure B1. 
Figure B1. 

Baseline municipality health characteristics and implementation year of the reform. We report results from separate regressions: Rjt=(YEARt×Xj)ζt+τt+ϵjt, where the dependent variable Rjt is an indicator of the timing of the reform (1 if the reform was implemented in year t in municipality j). Explanatory variables contain year fixed effects τt and the year dummies YEARt interacted with the outcome Xj indicated on the panel title (Hosp. = hospitalization). The outcomes are measured in 1971. The graphs plot the coefficients of the interaction terms ζt, together with 95% confidence intervals (based on robust standard errors clustered at the municipal level). Estimated coefficients have been divided by the standard deviation of the corresponding variable.

Figure B2. 
Figure B2. 

Baseline municipality characteristics and implementation year of the reform. See figure B1 legend. The municipal characteristics are measured in 1971 or the nearest available year.

Figure B3. 
Figure B3. 

Effect of the reform on mental health–related deaths at age 16–45: leads and lags around the year before the reform. Plots are based on baseline regression models, where the reform dummy is replaced with year dummies for the leads and lags around the reform year. The plotted points represent the estimates on the lead and lag dummies. The omitted category is the year before the reform (−1). The estimated effects are reported, together with their 95% confidence intervals (based on robust standard errors clustered at the municipal level).

Figure B4. 
Figure B4. 

Effect of the reform on all-cause mortality at age 16–45: leads and lags around the year before the reform. Plots are based on baseline regression models, where the reform dummy is replaced with year dummies for the leads and lags around the reform year. The plotted points represent the estimates on the lead and lag dummies. The omitted category is the year before the reform (−1). The estimated effects are reported, together with their 95% confidence intervals (based on robust standard errors clustered at the municipal level).

Table B2. 

Effect of the Reform on Mental Health–Related Deaths by Age 45

 Full SampleParental Education
LowMidHigh
 Males
Treatment effect−.0014−.0030a,**.0010−.0001
 (.0011)(.0014)(.0012)(.0012)
R2.0033.0056.0086.0147
Mean outcome.0187.0143.0051.0036
Observations186,77794,03747,22436,282
 Females
Treatment effect.0002−.0003.0004.0005
 (.0005)(.0006)(.0005)(.0008)
R2.0025.0050.0089.0129
Mean outcome.0043.0031.0012.0012
Observations179,53790,88146,09234,828

Note.  The full sample also includes individuals for whom information about parents (such as parental education) is missing. All models include cohort and municipality fixed effects. Standard errors, reported in parentheses, are clustered at the municipal level.

a Significance level after Romano-Wolf correction for multiple-hypothesis testing (Romano and Wolf 2005) is .104 (see table B11).

** Significant at the 5% level (two-sided test).

View Table Image
Table B3. 

Effect of the Reform on All-Cause Mortality by Age 45

 Full SampleParental Education
LowMidHigh
 Males
Treatment effect−.0016−.0034.0022−.0014
 (.0017)(.0022)(.0018)(.0017)
R2.0032.0064.0099.0118
Mean outcome.0458.0339.0132.0102
Observations186,77794,03747,22436,282
 Females
Treatment effect.0007−.0005.0009−.0008
 (.0011)(.0013)(.0011)(.0012)
R2.0025.0051.0098.0112
Mean outcome.0176.0122.0050.0047
Observations179,53790,88146,09234,828

Note.  The full sample also includes individuals for whom information about parents (such as parental education) is missing. All models include cohort and municipality fixed effects. Standard errors, reported in parentheses, are clustered at the municipal level. None of the coefficients are significant at the 10% level (two-sided tests).

View Table Image
Figure B5. 
Figure B5. 

Age-specific treatment effects on mental health–related hospitalizations by sex and parental background. Each age-specific treatment effect of comprehensive schooling is estimated from separate models. The dependent variable is a dummy variable indicating whether or not an individual was hospitalized as a result of mental health disorders during the specific age range (e.g., 6–10). The left-hand side of the vertical line denotes the pretreatment period. Treatment effects are reported, together with their 95% confidence intervals (based on robust standard errors clustered at the municipal level).

Figure B6. 
Figure B6. 

Illustration of the reform status in municipalities across NUTS-2 regions. Data are aggregated to the municipal level. Municipalities have been classified according to the NUTS-2 regional level. The figure illustrates the regional allocation of individuals with pre- and postreform schooling at age 11. For example, in the Helsinki and other southern Finland (outside Helsinki) regions, most municipalities have roughly the same number of individuals with pre- and postreform schooling. In northern and eastern Finland, the sample is not as well balanced between individuals with pre- and postreform schooling. Population sizes of the NUTS-2 regions range from 74,500 to 114,000 inhabitants. Average number of observations per municipality is 788 (median is 410).

Table B4. 

Estimated Reform Effect after Controlling for Alternative Regional Effects

 Full SampleParental Education
LowMidHigh
 A. Controls for the Six Years of Adoption
 Males
Suicide by age 45−.0001−.0015.0015.0005
 (.0008)(.0011)(.0010)(.0009)
Mental health–related death by age 45−.0013−.0027**.0011.0002
 (.0011)(.0013)(.0012)(.0011)
Death by age 45−.0012−.0028.0030*−.0011
 (.0017)(.0022)(.0018)(.0017)
Mental health–related hospitalizations at age 16–45.0029.0079**.0011−.0052
 (.0025)(.0036)(.0045)(.0042)
 Females
Suicide by age 45−.0002−.0005−.0001.0004
 (.0005)(.0005)(.0004)(.0007)
Mental health–related death by age 45.0003−.0003.0003.0007
 (.0005)(.0006)(.0005)(.0008)
Death by age 45.0007−.0004.0008−.0007
 (.0011)(.0013)(.0011)(.0012)
Mental health–related hospitalizations at age 16–45.0031*.0024−.0004.0101**
 (.0019)(.0025)(.0034)(.0040)
 B. Controls for the 18 NUTS-3 Regions
 Males
Suicide by age 45.0001−.0014.0007.0011
 (.0008)(.0009)(.0008)(.0009)
Mental health–related death by age 45−.0009−.0026**−.0002.0009
 (.0010)(.0011)(.0010)(.0011)
Death by age 45−.0010−.0033*.0009−.0007
 (.0015)(.0018)(.0016)(.0014)
Mental health–related hospitalizations at age 16–45.0003.0036−.0059−.0055
 (.0028)(.0039)(.0041)(.0036)
 Females
Suicide by age 45−.0003−.0006−.0002.0002
 (.0004)(.0004)(.0003)(.0006)
Mental health–related death by age 45−.0002−.0009*−.0000.0005
 (.0004)(.0005)(.0004)(.0007)
Death by age 45.0002−.0013.0002−.0007
 (.0009)(.0011)(.0010)(.0011)
Mental health–related hospitalizations at age 16–45−.0003−.0014−.0017.0043
 (.0016)(.0024)(.0030)(.0037)

Note.  Each cell reports the estimated reform effect from separate models. In panel A, we have replaced the full municipal fixed effects with six dummies for the years of adoption (1972–77). In panel B, we have replaced the municipal fixed effects with 18 NUTS-3 regional dummies. All models include cohort fixed effects. Standard errors, reported in parentheses, are clustered at the municipal level.

* Significant at the 10% level (all two-sided tests).

** Significant at the 5% level (all two-sided tests).

View Table Image: 1 | 2
Table B5. 

Estimated Effect of the Reform after Controlling for Month of Birth

 Full SampleParental Education
LowMidHigh
 A. Controlling for Birth Month
 Males
Suicide by age 45−.0002−.0016.0013.0003
 (.0008)(.0011)(.0010)(.0010)
Mental health–related death by age 45−.0014−.0030**.0010−.0001
 (.0011)(.0013)(.0012)(.0012)
Death by age 45−.0016−.0034.0022−.0014
 (.0017)(.0022)(.0018)(.0017)
Mental health–related hospitalizations at age 16–45.0027.0074**.0012−.0058
 (.0025)(.0036)(.0045)(.0043)
 Females
Suicide by age 45−.0003−.0006−.0001.0002
 (.0005)(.0005)(.0004)(.0007)
Mental health–related death by age 45.0002−.0003.0004.0005
 (.0005)(.0006)(.0005)(.0008)
Death by age 45.0007−.0006.0009−.0008
 (.0011)(.0013)(.0011)(.0012)
Mental health–related hospitalizations at age 16–45.0029.0023−.0007.0096**
 (.0019)(.0025)(.0034)(.0041)
 B. Controlling for Birth Month and Its Interaction with Birth Year
 Males
Suicide by age 45−.0002−.0016.0013.0003
 (.0008)(.0011)(.0010)(.0010)
Mental health–related death by age 45−.0013−.0030**.0010−.0001
 (.0011)(.0013)(.0012)(.0012)
Death by age 45−.0016−.0034.0022−.0014
 (.0017)(.0022)(.0018)(.0017)
Mental health–related hospitalizations at age 16–45.0028.0075**.0012−.0060
 (.0025)(.0036)(.0045)(.0043)
 Females
Suicide by age 45−.0003−.0006−.0001.0002
 (.0005)(.0005)(.0004)(.0007)
Mental health–related death by age 45.0002−.0003.0004.0005
 (.0005)(.0006)(.0005)(.0008)
Death by age 45.0007−.0006.0008−.0009
 (.0011)(.0013)(.0011)(.0012)
Mental health–related hospitalizations at age 16–45.0029.0024−.0006.0096**
 (.0019)(.0025)(.0034)(.0041)

Note.  Each cell reports the estimated effect of the reform from separate models. All models include cohort as well as municipality fixed effects. Standard errors, reported in parentheses, are clustered at the municipal level.

** Significant at the 5% level (all two-sided tests).

View Table Image: 1 | 2
Table B6. 

Timing of Schooling Relative to the Year of Adoption of the Reform in the Municipality of Residence

Birth cohortAdoption Year in the Municipality
197219731974197519761977
1960−1−2−3−4−5−6
19610−1−2−3−4−5
196210−1−2−3−4
1963210−1−2−3
19643210−1−2
196543210−1
1966543210

Note.  For each birth cohort, people affected by the reform were those who lived in municipalities where the reform was adopted in the years in boldface font. People who were born in 1965 and lived in a region where the reform was adopted in 1974 entered the fifth grade 2 years after the first postreform schooling cohort in the region. Also see figure 2.

View Table Image: 1 | 2
Table B7. 

Robustness Checks of the Estimated Effect of the Reform on Suicide

 Full SampleParental Education
LowMidHigh
 Males
Baseline results−.0002−.0016.0013.0003
 (.0008)(.0011)(.0010)(.0010)
Controlling for prior mental health disorders−.0002−.0016.0013.0003
 (.0008)(.0011)(.0010)(.0010)
Controlling for prior mental health and parents’ mental health−.0002−.0016.0013.0003
 (.0008)(.0011)(.0010)(.0010)
Controlling for prior mental health disorders and their interaction with treatment status−.0002−.0016.0013.0003
 (.0008)(.0011)(.0010)(.0010)
Controlling for region-specific linear time trends−.0010−.0008.0008−.0002
 (.0009)(.0013)(.0012)(.0012)
Excluding Helsinki metropolitan area−.0008−.0023*.0019*−.0011
 (.0010)(.0013)(.0011)(.0012)
Excluding if t < −3 or t > 3−.0003−.0011.0019*.0001
 (.0009)(.0012)(.0011)(.0010)
Extending birth cohorts to 1960–66, excluding if t < −3 or t > 3.0002−.0010.0020**.0005
 (.0009)(.0011)(.0010)(.0010)
 Females
Baseline results−.0003−.0006−.0001.0002
 (.0005)(.0005)(.0004)(.0007)
Controlling for prior mental health disorders−.0003−.0006−.0001.0002
 (.0005)(.0005)(.0004)(.0007)
Controlling for prior mental health and parents’ mental health−.0003−.0006−.0001.0002
 (.0005)(.0005)(.0004)(.0007)
Controlling for prior mental health disorders and their interaction with treatment status−.0003−.0006−.0001.0002
 (.0005)(.0005)(.0004)(.0007)
Controlling for region-specific linear time trends−.0006−.0006−.0003.0002
 (.0006)(.0006)(.0005)(.0009)
Excluding Helsinki metropolitan area−.0006−.0010−.0002.0007
 (.0005)(.0006)(.0005)(.0009)
Excluding if t < −3 or t > 3−.0010*−.0012**−.0002−.0001
 (.0005)(.0006)(.0005)(.0009)
Extending birth cohorts to 1960–66, excluding if t < −3 or t > 3−.0007−.0010**−.0002.0004
 (.0005)(.0005)(.0005)(.0008)

Note.  Baseline results are those reported in table 3. All specifications include cohort and municipality fixed effects. Standard errors, reported in parentheses, are clustered at the municipal level. t is the number of years relative to the reform year in the municipality of residence (see table B6).

* Significant at the 10% level (all two-sided tests).

** Significant at the 5% level (all two-sided tests).

View Table Image: 1 | 2
Table B8. 

Robustness Checks of the Estimated Effect of the Reform on Mental Health–Related Deaths

 Full SampleParental Education
LowMidHigh
 Males
Baseline results−.0014−.0030**.0010−.0001
 (.0011)(.0014)(.0012)(.0012)
Controlling for prior mental health disorders−.0014−.0030**.0010−.0001
 (.0011)(.0014)(.0012)(.0012)
Controlling for prior mental health and parents’ mental health−.0014−.0030**.0010−.0001
 (.0011)(.0013)(.0012)(.0012)
Controlling for prior mental health disorders and their interaction with treatment status−.0014−.0030**.0010−.0001
 (.0011)(.0014)(.0012)(.0012)
Controlling for region-specific linear time trends−.0019−.0017.0004−.0013
 (.0013)(.0016)(.0015)(.0014)
Excluding Helsinki metropolitan area−.0026*−.0043***.0019−.0012
 (.0013)(.0016)(.0013)(.0015)
Excluding if t < −3 or t > 3−.0013−.0024.0020−.0005
 (.0012)(.0016)(.0013)(.0013)
Extending birth cohorts to 1960–66, excluding if t < −3 or t > 3−.0009−.0023.0020*−.0002
 (.0011)(.0014)(.0012)(.0012)
 Females
Baseline results.0002−.0003.0004.0005
 (.0005)(.0006)(.0005)(.0008)
Controlling for prior mental health disorders.0002−.0003.0004.0005
 (.0005)(.0006)(.0005)(.0008)
Controlling for prior mental health and parents’ mental health.0002−.0003.0004.0005
 (.0005)(.0006)(.0005)(.0008)
Controlling for prior mental health disorders and their interaction with treatment status.0002−.0004.0004.0005
 (.0005)(.0006)(.0005)(.0008)
Controlling for region-specific linear time trends.0001−.0001.0004.0008
 (.0006)(.0007)(.0006)(.0010)
Excluding Helsinki metropolitan area−.0001−.0008.0002.0011
 (.0006)(.0007)(.0006)(.0010)
Excluding if t < −3 or t > 3−.0006−.0009.0002.0005
 (.0006)(.0008)(.0006)(.0010)
Extending birth cohorts to 1960–66, excluding if t < −3 or t > 3−.0003−.0009.0003.0011
 (.0006)(.0006)(.0006)(.0009)

Note.  Baseline results are those reported in table B2. All specifications include cohort and municipality fixed effects. Standard errors, reported in parentheses, are clustered at the municipal level. t is timing of the reform in the number of years relative to the reform year in the municipality of residence (see table B6).

* Significant at the 10% level (all two-sided tests).

** Significant at the 5% level (all two-sided tests).

*** Significant at the 1% level (all two-sided tests).

View Table Image: 1 | 2
Table B9. 

Robustness Checks of the Estimated Effect of the Reform on All-Cause Mortality

 Full SampleParental Education
LowMidHigh
 Males
Baseline results−.0016−.0034.0022−.0014
 (.0017)(.0022)(.0018)(.0017)
Controlling for prior mental health disorders−.0016−.0034.0023−.0014
 (.0017)(.0022)(.0018)(.0017)
Controlling for prior mental health and parents’ mental health−.0016−.0034.0022−.0014
 (.0017)(.0022)(.0018)(.0017)
Controlling for prior mental health disorders and their interaction with treatment status−.0014−.0033.0023−.0014
 (.0017)(.0022)(.0018)(.0017)
Controlling for region-specific linear time trends−.0022−.0007.0008−.0019
 (.0020)(.0026)(.0023)(.0020)
Excluding Helsinki metropolitan area−.0016−.0042.0037*−.0018
 (.0020)(.0026)(.0020)(.0021)
Excluding if t < −3 or t > 3−.0010−.0014.0035*.0001
 (.0019)(.0024)(.0019)(.0017)
Extending birth cohorts to 1960–66, excluding if t < −3 or t > 3.0000−.0011.0043**.0015
 (.0018)(.0023)(.0019)(.0018)
 Females
Baseline results.0007−.0005.0009−.0008
 (.0011)(.0013)(.0011)(.0012)
Controlling for prior mental health disorders.0007−.0005.0009−.0008
 (.0011)(.0013)(.0011)(.0012)
Controlling for prior mental health and parents’ mental health.0007−.0005.0009−.0008
 (.0011)(.0013)(.0011)(.0012)
Controlling for prior mental health disorders and their interaction with treatment status.0007−.0007.0010−.0009
 (.0011)(.0013)(.0011)(.0012)
Controlling for region-specific linear time trends.0020*.0015.0010−.0002
 (.0012)(.0017)(.0013)(.0014)
Excluding Helsinki metropolitan area.0003−.0020.0013−.0006
 (.0013)(.0015)(.0013)(.0016)
Excluding if t < −3 or t > 3.0002−.0005.0005−.0013
 (.0011)(.0015)(.0012)(.0014)
Extending birth cohorts to 1960–66, excluding if t < −3 or t > 3.0001−.0001.0003−.0010
 (.0011)(.0013)(.0012)(.0013)

Note.  Baseline results are those reported in table B3. All specifications include cohort and municipality fixed effects. Standard errors, reported in parentheses, are clustered at the municipal level. t is timing of the reform in number of years relative to the reform year in the municipality of residence (see table B6).

* Significant at the 10% level (all two-sided tests).

** Significant at the 5% level (all two-sided tests).

View Table Image: 1 | 2
Table B10. 

Robustness Checks of the Estimated Effect of the Reform on Mental Health–Related Hospitalizations at Age 16–45

 Full SampleParental Education
LowMidHigh
 Males
Baseline results.0027.0074**.0012−.0058
 (.0025)(.0036)(.0045)(.0043)
Controlling for prior mental health disorders.0028.0075**.0014−.0057
 (.0025)(.0036)(.0045)(.0043)
Controlling for prior mental health and parents’ mental health.0029.0078**.0012−.0056
 (.0025)(.0036)(.0046)(.0043)
Controlling for prior mental health disorders and their interaction with treatment status.0029.0076**.0018−.0057
 (.0025)(.0036)(.0046)(.0042)
Controlling for region-specific linear time trends−.0012.0030−.0011−.0043
 (.0030)(.0045)(.0053)(.0048)
Excluding Helsinki metropolitan area.0032.0051.0049−.0078
 (.0026)(.0038)(.0051)(.0057)
Excluding if t < −3 or t > 3.0024.0068*.0022−.0037
 (.0028)(.0041)(.0050)(.0045)
Extending birth cohorts to 1960–66, excluding if t < −3 or t > 3.0018.0039.0046−.0017
 (.0024)(.0036)(.0046)(.0043)
 Females
Baseline results.0029.0022−.0007.0095**
 (.0019)(.0025)(.0034)(.0041)
Controlling for prior mental health disorders.0030.0023−.0007.0097**
 (.0019)(.0025)(.0034)(.0041)
Controlling for prior mental health and parents’ mental health.0029.0022−.0007.0097**
 (.0019)(.0025)(.0034)(.0041)
Controlling for prior mental health disorders and their interaction with treatment status.0030.0022−.0009.0100**
 (.0018)(.0025)(.0034)(.0041)
Controlling for region-specific linear time trends.0023.0002−.0006.0114**
 (.0024)(.0031)(.0042)(.0047)
Excluding Helsinki metropolitan area.0027.0027−.0018.0099*
 (.0021)(.0028)(.0040)(.0054)
Excluding if t < −3 or t > 3.0039*.0028.0014.0120***
 (.0021)(.0029)(.0038)(.0042)
Extending birth cohorts to 1960–66, excluding if t < −3 or t > 3.0039**.0035.0013.0092**
 (.0019)(.0026)(.0037)(.0039)

Note.  Baseline results are those reported in table 4. All specifications include cohort and municipality fixed effects. Standard errors, reported in parentheses, are clustered at the municipal level. t is the number of years relative to the reform year in the municipality of residence (see table B6).

* Significant at the 10% level (all two-sided tests).

** Significant at the 5% level (all two-sided tests).

*** Significant at the 1% level (all two-sided tests).

View Table Image: 1 | 2
Table B11. 

Significance Levels for the Main Results after Adjustment for Multiple-Hypothesis Testing

OutcomeSampleReform EffectConventional p-ValueRomano-Wolf p-Value
A. Main outcomes:    
 Mental health–related death by age 45Males, low-educated parents−.0030.025.104
 Mental health–related hospitalizations at ages 16–45Males, low-educated parents.0074.039.146
 Mental health–related hospitalizations at ages 16–45Females, highly educated parents.0095.020.083
B. Timing of hospitalization:   
 Ages 26–35Males, low-educated parents.0062.005.022
 Ages 36–45Males, low-educated parents.0046.052.183
 Ages 11–15Females, mid-educated parents.0024.014.048
 Ages 36–45Females, highly educated parents.0058.039.144
C. Type of mental disorder:   
 Substance-use disorderMales, full sample.0038.017.062
 Substance-use disorderMales, low-educated parents.0048.038.106
 Bipolar disorderFemales, highly educated parents.0026.058.205
 Depressive disorderFemales, highly educated parents.0062.005.022

Note.  We report only estimated reform effects that are significant in tables 36, B2, and B3. Conventional and Romano-Wolf step-down adjusted p-values are based on standard errors that are clustered at the municipal level. The adjusted p-values are robust to multiple-hypothesis testing (jointly for the full sample and the three subsamples by parental background). The adjusted p-values have been calculated using 2,000 bootstrap replications.

View Table Image
Table B12. 

Multiple-Hypotheses Testing with Alternative Romano-Wolf (RW) p-Values

OutcomeSampleBaseline RW p-Value
(1)
RW p-Value with 3 Subsamples
(2)
RW p-value with 6 Subsamples
(3)
A. Main outcomes:    
 Mental health–related death by age 45Males, low-educated parents.104.075.145
 Mental health–related hospitalizations at ages 16–45Males, low-educated parents.146.112.185
 Mental health–related hospitalizations at ages 16–45Females, highly educated parents.083.059.119
B. Timing of hospitalization:   
 Ages 26–35Males, low-educated parents.022.021.038
 Ages 36–45Males, low-educated parents.183.147.232
 Ages 11–15Females, mid-educated parents.048.044.065
 Ages 36–45Females, highly educated parents.144.102.217
C. Type of mental disorder:   
 Substance-use disorderMales, full sample.062NANA
 Substance-use disorderMales, low-educated parents.106.102.207
 Bipolar disorderFemales, highly educated parents.205.165.288
 Depressive disorderFemales, highly educated parents.022.015.035

Note.  We report only estimated reform effects that are significant in tables 36, B2, and B3. RW step-down adjusted p-values are based on standard errors that are clustered at the municipal level. The adjusted p-values have been calculated using 2,000 bootstrap replications. In col. 1, the baseline RW p-values have been computed for the full sample and the three subsamples by parental background (see table B11). In col. 2, RW p-values have been computed for the three subsamples by parental background only. In col. 3, RW p-values have been computed for all the six subsamples by parental background and gender. NA = not applicable.

View Table Image
Table B13. 

Significance Levels for the Main Results after Clustering at the NUTS-4 Regional Level

OutcomeSampleReform EffectConventional p-ValueRegional Clustering p-Value
A. Main outcomes:    
 Mental health–related death by age 45Males, low-educated parents−.0030.025.066
 Mental health–related hospitalizations at ages 16–45Males, low-educated parents.0074.039.058
 Mental health–related hospitalizations at ages 16–45Females, highly educated parents.0095.020.015
B. Timing of hospitalization:   
 Ages 26–35Males, low-educated parents.0062.005.003
 Ages 36–45Males, low-educated parents.0046.052.055
 Ages 11–15Females, mid-educated parents.0024.014.028
 Ages 36–45Females, highly educated parents.0058.039.057
C. Type of mental disorder:   
 Substance-use disorderMales, full sample.0038.017.007
 Substance-use disorderMales, low-educated parents.0048.038.037
 Bipolar disorderFemales, highly educated parents.0026.058.014
 Depressive disorderFemales, highly educated parents.0062.005.004

Note.  We report only estimated reform effects that are significant in tables 36, B2, and B3. Conventional p-values are based on standard errors that are clustered at the municipal level (465 municipalities). Alternative p-values utilize clustering at the NUTS-4 regional level (i.e., municipalities aggregated to 67 regions). The precision of the estimates does not improve by introduction of an additional control for parents’ income (see table B14).

View Table Image: 1 | 2
Table B14. 

Main Results after Controlling for Parents’ Income

OutcomeSampleReform Effect in the Baseline (with p-Value)After Controlling for Parents’ Income (with p-Value)
A. Main outcomes:   
 Mental health–related death by age 45Males, low-educated parents−.0030−.0031
  (.025)(.020)
 Mental health–related hospitalizations at ages 16–45Males, low-educated parents.0074.0071
  (.039)(.051)
 Mental health–related hospitalizations at ages 16–45Females, highly educated parents.0095.0095
  (.020)(.020)
B. Timing of hospitalization:  
 Ages 26–35Males, low-educated parents.0062.0060
  (.005)(.007)
 Ages 36–45Males, low-educated parents.0046.0043
  (.052)(.064)
 Ages 11–15Females, mid-educated parents.0024.0024
  (.014)(.015)
 Ages 36–45Females, highly educated parents.0058.0056
  (.039)(.038)
C. Type of mental disorder:  
 Substance-use disorderMales, full sample.0038.0036
  (.017)(.020)
 Substance-use disorderMales, low-educated parents.0048.0046
  (.038)(.048)
 Bipolar disorderFemales, highly educated parents.0026.0026
  (.058)(.057)
 Depressive disorderFemales, highly educated parents.0062.0062
  (.005)(.005)

Note.  We report only estimated reform effects that are significant in tables 36, A2, and A3. Each cell reports the estimated effect of the comprehensive school reform from separate models. All models include cohort as well as municipality fixed effects. Additional controls are parents’ income percentile (0–100) within the child’s birth year and whether this information is missing. Parents’ income is computed as the average over measurement from 1975 and 1985 for nonmissing years and for nonmissing parents. The p-values, reported in parentheses, are based on standard errors clustered at the municipal level.

View Table Image: 1 | 2
Table B15. 

Estimated Effect of the Reform in the Combined Sample of Males and Females

OutcomeFull SampleParental Education
LowMidHigh
A. Main outcomes:    
 Suicide by age 45−.0002−.0011*.0007.0004
 (.0005)(.0006)(.0005)(.0006)
 Mental health–related death by age 45−.0006−.0016**.0007.0003
 (.0006)(.0007)(.0006)(.0007)
 Death by age 45−.0005−.0019.0018*−.0009
 (.0010)(.0013)(.0010)(.0011)
 Mental health–related hospitalizations at ages 16–45.0028*.0051**.0002.0023
 (.0015)(.0022)(.0026)(.0030)
B. Timing of hospitalization:   
 Ages 6–10−.0006−.0005−.0007−.0013
 (.0004)(.0007)(.0007)(.0008)
 Ages 11–15.0002.0001.0004−.0016***
 (.0004)(.0006)(.0008)(.0006)
 Ages 16–25.0015.0025−.0013.0021
 (.0011)(.0016)(.0020)(.0018)
 Ages 26–35.0021**.0038***.0018−.0012
 (.0010)(.0013)(.0020)(.0023)
 Ages 36–45.0015.0033**−.0026.0016
 (.0011)(.0015)(.0020)(.0021)
C. Type of mental disorder:   
 Schizophrenia.0004.0015*−.0006.0002
 (.0006)(.0008)(.0011)(.0014)
 Other nonaffective.0001.0014−.0011−.0010
 (.0007)(.0010)(.0014)(.0014)
 Bipolar disorder.0006*.0006.0005.0007
 (.0004)(.0005)(.0007)(.0010)
 Depressive disorder.0014**.0014.0007.0013
 (.0007)(.0010)(.0014)(.0014)
 Anxiety, stress, neurotic.0006.0015−.0005−.0010
 (.0008)(.0010)(.0012)(.0011)
 Substance-use disorder.0025***.0036***.0002.0025*
 (.0009)(.0013)(.0017)(.0015)
Observations366,314184,91893,31671,110

Note.  Each cell reports the estimated effect of the comprehensive school reform from separate models. All models include gender dummy as well as cohort and municipality fixed effects. Standard errors, reported in parentheses, are clustered at the municipal level.

* Significant at the 10% level (all two-sided tests).

** Significant at the 5% level (all two-sided tests).

*** Significant at the 1% level (all two-sided tests).

View Table Image: 1 | 2
Table B16. 

Cox Proportional Hazards Model Results for All-Cause Mortality

 Full SampleParental Education
LowMidHigh
 Males
Treatment effect.965.8991.191.865
 (.036)(.059)(.160)(.143)
 [.896, 1.038][.789, 1.023][.915, 1.548][.625, 1.196]
Observations186,77794,03747,22436,282
 Females
Treatment effect1.035.9531.257.834
 (.063)(.102)(.282)(.213)
 [.919, 1.166][.774, 1.174][.810, 1.951][.506, 1.375]
Observations179,53790,88146,09234,828

Note.  Birth cohorts 1962–66 are at risk from age 11 until age 45. Annual death hazard ratios are reported, together with 95% confidence intervals in square brackets. Standard errors, reported in parentheses, are clustered at the municipal level. Hazard ratios greater than 1 indicate increased mortality. All specifications include birth cohort dummies, and the estimates have been stratified by municipality (i.e., holding the baseline hazard constant within municipality). In a combined sample of males and females, the treatment effect is 0.985 (0.030), with 95% confidence interval [0.927, 1.046].

View Table Image
Table B17. 

Estimated Effect of the Reform on the Number of Days Spent in Hospital for Mental Health Reasons between Ages 16 and 45

 Full SampleParental Education
LowMidHigh
 Males
Hospitalization days3.1446**4.1429**1.1525.5366
 (1.4899)(2.0915)(2.5885)(3.2623)
 Mean outcome (days)15.0215.4712.4813.71
 Observations186,77794,03747,22436,282
ln(Hospitalization days) conditional on being hospitalized.0631.1248**−.0593.1577
 (.0401)(.0626)(.1080)(.1460)
 Mean outcome (days)150.67143.63147.29186.49
 Observations18,61610,1304,0012,668
 Females
Hospitalization days1.38532.6531−.7588−.5380
 (1.1449)(1.8333)(1.8682)(2.6052)
 Mean outcome (days)10.8610.959.26011.78
 Observations179,53790,88146,09234,828
ln(Hospitalization days) conditional on being hospitalized.0057.1210−.0442−.2036
 (.0683)(.0972)(.1427)(.1595)
 Mean outcome (days)192.59184.60186.01226.15
 Observations10,1255,3892,2951,814

Note.  Each cell reports the estimated effect of the comprehensive school reform from separate models. All models include cohort and municipality fixed effects. Standard errors reported in parenthesis are clustered at the municipal level.

** Significant at the 5% level (all two-sided tests).

View Table Image
Table B18. 

Effect of the Reform: Treatment Assignment According to the Municipality of Birth

OutcomeFull SampleParental Education
LowMidHigh
 Males
Suicide by age 45−.0004−.0017.0016*−.0010
 (.0009)(.0011)(.0009)(.0008)
Mental health–related death by age 45−.0008−.0028**.0016−.0010
 (.0010)(.0013)(.0012)(.0010)
Death by age 45−.0014−.0026.0027−.0033*
 (.0016)(.0020)(.0019)(.0018)
MHD by age 45.0021.0039.0048−.0071*
 (.0027)(.0035)(.0052)(.0039)
Observations194,02097,36648,53037,554
 Females
Suicide by age 45−.0002−.0001−.0001.0005
 (.0004)(.0005)(.0004)(.0005)
Mental health–related death by age 45.0002−.0001.0007.0007
 (.0005)(.0005)(.0005)(.0005)
Death by age 45.0007.0000.0016−.0002
 (.0010)(.0011)(.0010)(.0012)
MHD by age 45.0002−.0005−.0022.0066*
 (.0017)(.0026)(.0032)(.0037)
Observations186,62994,13847,36336,075

Note.  Here, individuals are classified into treatment on the basis of their municipality of birth instead of municipality at age 11 (see results in tables 3, 4, B2, B3, B19). Individuals who migrate between ages 11 and 16 are not dropped from the sample. MHD = Mental health disorders requiring hospitalization. The full sample also includes individuals for whom information about parents (such as parental education) is missing. All models include cohort and municipality of birth fixed effects. Standard errors, reported in parentheses, are clustered at the municipal level.

* Significant at the 10% level (all two-sided tests).

** Significant at the 5% level (all two-sided tests).

View Table Image: 1 | 2
Table B19. 

Effect of the Reform: Treatment Assignment According to the Municipality at Age 10

OutcomeFull SampleParental Education
LowMidHigh
 Males
Suicide by age 45−.0003−.0015.0013.0000
 (.0006)(.0011)(.0010)(.0011)
Mental health–related death by age 45−.0010−.0027**.0012−.0001
 (.0008)(.0013)(.0012)(.0012)
Death by age 45−.0004−.0023.0030*−.0012
 (.0013)(.0022)(.0018)(.0017)
MHD by age 45.0026.0065*.0000−.0055
 (.0024)(.0034)(.0047)(.0041)
Observations181,13695,62048,18937,327
 Females
Suicide by age 45−.0003−.0006−.0003.0003
 (.0003)(.0005)(.0004)(.0007)
Mental health–related death by age 45.0001−.0002.0004.0005
 (.0004)(.0006)(.0005)(.0008)
Death by age 45−.0001−.0004.0009−.0008
 (.0007)(.0013)(.0011)(.0012)
MHD by age 45.0023.0021−.0023.0080**
 (.0019)(.0025)(.0035)(.0039)
Observations175,30492,46546,99835,841

Note.  Here, individuals are classified into treatment on the basis of their municipality at age 10 instead of municipality at age 11 (see results in tables 3, 4, B2, B3, and B18). Individuals who migrate between ages 11 and 16 have not been dropped from the sample. MHD = Mental health disorders requiring hospitalization. The full sample also includes individuals for whom information about parents (such as parental education) is missing. All models include cohort and municipality-at-age-10 fixed effects. Standard errors, reported in parentheses, are clustered at the municipal level.

* Significant at the 10% level (all two-sided tests).

** Significant at the 5% level (all two-sided tests).

View Table Image
Table B20. 

Estimated Effect of the Reform on Educational Outcomes

OutcomeFull SampleParental Education
LowMidHigh
 Males
Years of schooling−.0489**−.0630**−.1384***.0229
 (.0222)(.0266)(.0416)(.0562)
 [12.777][12.183][12.790][14.577]
Has a high school degree−.0022−.0020−.0163**−.0020
 (.0037)(.0050)(.0068)(.0082)
 [.291][.177][.265][.636]
High school exam score (percentile):    
 In native language.0007−.0047−.0036.0047
 (.0059)(.0080)(.0093)(.0086)
 [.463][.431][.429][.503]
 In advanced math−.0173**−.0104−.0370**−.0147
 (.0073)(.0128)(.0164)(.0094)
 [.538][.500][.511][.571]
 In basic math−.0103.0067−.0329**.0005
 (.0070)(.0132)(.0144)(.0117)
 [.506][.485][.493][.535]
Highest degree completed:    
 Vocational secondary education−.0005−.0073.0143**.0040
 (.0042)(.0067)(.0066)(.0074)
 [.459][.536][.502][.232]
 Vocational college.0020.0041.0005−.0027
 (.0027)(.0038)(.0057)(.0069)
 [.140][.116][.157][.192]
 University master’s−.0048*−.0039−.0151***.0015
 (.0027)(.0028)(.0049)(.0086)
 [.113][.055][.090][.301]
 Females
Years of schooling−.0139−.0329.0157.0126
 (.0207)(.0258)(.0443)(.0498)
 [13.489][12.978][13.532][14.977]
Has a high school degree−.0082**−.0109**.0024−.0107
 (.0041)(.0053)(.0093)(.0077)
 [.498][.383][.505][.790]
High school exam score (percentile):    
 In native language−.0083−.0084−.0190**.0019
 (.0055)(.0060)(.0086)(.0078)
 [.497][.457][.472][.568]
 In advanced math−.0137*−.0206*−.0242*−.0063
 (.0071)(.0124)(.0123)(.0111)
 [.484][.447][.455[.528]
 In basic math−.0122**−.0229***−.0241**.0159*
 (.0061)(.0082)(.0105)(.0084)
 [.502][.472][.487][.556]
Highest degree completed:    
 Vocational secondary education−.0049−.0056−.0174**.0075
 (.0045)(.0059)(.0081)(.0074)
 [.379][.454][.394][.194]
 Vocational college.0021.0011.0110−.0052
 (.0042)(.0053)(.0074)(.0095)
 [.254][.244][.275][.264]
 University master’s−.0025−.0070**.0032.0043
 (.0032)(.0035)(.0054)(.0093)
 [.148][.086][.131][.337]

Note.  Each cell reports the estimated effect of the comprehensive school reform from separate models. High school exam score is available only for those who have graduated from a high school. All models include dummy variables for the year of reform and municipality fixed effects. Standard errors, reported in parentheses, are clustered at the municipal level. Mean values of dependent variables are reported in square brackets below the standard error. The full sample also includes individuals for whom information about parents (such as parental education) is missing.

* Significant at the 10% level (all two-sided tests).

** Significant at the 5% level (all two-sided tests).

*** Significant at the 1% level (all two-sided tests).

View Table Image: 1 | 2
Table B21. 

Estimated Reform Effect on Economic Outcomes

OutcomeFull SampleParental Education
LowMidHigh
 Males
Log of average income:    
 At ages 26–45−.0092*−.0187***−.0016.0022
 (.0050)(.0065)(.0089)(.0113)
 [3.307][3.234][3.332][3.528]
 At ages 26–35−.0022−.0144**.0096.0120
 (.0048)(.0057)(.0090)(.0097)
 [3.111][3.063][3.135][3.247]
 At ages 36–45−.0160***−.0234***−.0121−.0048
 (.0060)(.0080)(.0107)(.0137)
 [3.444][3.350][3.462][3.707]
Employment rate:    
 At ages 26–45−.0029−.0057**−.0004.0044
 (.0025)(.0031)(.0044)(.0046)
 [.766][.765][.806][.815]
 At ages 26–35−.0017−.0052*−.0002.0067
 (.0027)(.0031)(.0054)(.0049)
 [.740][.739][.775][.776]
 At ages 36–45−.0047−.0077**−.0008.0026
 (.0027)(.0036)(.0046)(.0050)
 [.803][.799][.841][.858]
 Females
Log of average income:    
 At ages 26–45−.0083**−.0145***−.0038.0018
 (.0039)(.0049)(.0078)(.0103)
 [3.047][2.998][3.069][3.204]
 At ages 26–35−.0036−.0079−.0014.0025
 (.0039)(.0049)(.0071)(.0097)
 [2.861][2.825][2.876][2.977]
 At ages 36–45−.0140***−.0211***−.0102−.0006
 (.0046)(.0064)(.0093)(.0116)
 [3.178][3.113][3.198][3.351]
Employment rate:    
 At ages 26–45−.0060**−.0066**−.0029−.0015
 (.0025)(.0029)(.0046)(.0055)
 [.726][.732].760][.764]
 At ages 26–35−.0034−.0056*−.0000.0012
 (.0025)(.0033)(.0049)(.0059)
 [.673][.668][.701][.704]
 At ages 36–45−.0085***−.0078**−.0060−.0050
 (.0031)(.0035)(.0055)(.0059)
 [.782][.790][.821][.824]

Note.  Each cell reports the estimated effect of the comprehensive school reform from separate models. All models include cohort and municipality fixed effects. Standard errors, reported in parentheses, are clustered at the municipal level. Mean values of dependent variables are reported in square brackets below the standard error. The full sample also includes individuals for whom information about parents (such as parental education) is missing.

* Significant at the 10% level (all two-sided tests).

** Significant at the 5% level (all two-sided tests).

*** Significant at the 1% level (all two-sided tests).

View Table Image: 1 | 2
Table B22. 

Significance Levels for the Mediator Results after Adjustment for Multiple-Hypothesis Testing

OutcomeMediation Analysis
Education
[with RW adjusted p-Value]
Education and Income
[with RW adjusted p-Value]
 A. Males, Low-Educated Parents
Suicide by age 45−.0018−.0017
 [.337][.285]
Mental health–related death by age 45−.0033−.0027
 [.0620][.1139]
Death by age 45−.0040−.0042
 [.241][.144]
MHD at ages 16–45.0061.0060
 [.301][.367]
MHD at ages 26–35.0056.0049
 [.045][.118]
MHD at ages 36–45.0038.0032
 [.354][.524]
Substance-use disorder at ages 16–45.0041.0033
 [.184][.370]
 B. Females, Highly Educated Parents
Suicide by age 45.0003.0005
 [.925][.843]
Mental health–related death by age 45.0005.0007
 [.897][.711]
Death by age 45−.0008−.0008
 [.861][.868]
MHD at ages 16–45.0096.0097
 [.092][.086]
MHD at ages 26–35.0026.0025
 [.633][.654]
MHD at ages 36–45.0057.0061
 [.147][.113]
Depression at ages 16–45.0063.0065
 [.022][.017]

Note.  We present controlled direct effects based on Acharya, Blackwell, and Sen (2016), after controlling for education and/or income. The Romano-Wolf (RW) step-down adjusted p-values, reported in square brackets, are robust to multiple hypotheses testing (jointly for the full sample and the three subsamples by parental background). They have been computed using 2,000 bootstrap replications. Significance levels are based on standard errors that are clustered at the municipal level. MHD = Mental health disorders requiring hospitalization. See also table 7.

View Table Image: 1 | 2
Table B23. 

Estimated Reform Effect, Controlling for Education and Income Mediators Later in Life (by Gender)

OutcomeBaseline Estimates
(1)
Mediation Analysis
Education
(2)
Education and Income
(3)
 A. Males
Suicide by age 45−.0002−.0004−.0004
 (.0008)(.0008)(.0007)
Mental health–related death by age 45−.0014−.0016−.0015
 (.0011)(.0011)(.0010)
Death by age 45−.0016−.0022−.0022
 (.0017)(.0017)(.0016)
MHD at ages 16–45.0027.0018.0021
 (.0025)(.0026)(.0025)
MHD at ages 26–35.0020.0016.0016
 (.0016)(.0016)(.0016)
MHD at ages 36–45.0018.0013.0014
 (.0016)(.0016)(.0016)
Substance-use disorder at ages 16–45.0038**.0033**.0031*
 (.0016)(.0016)(.0016)
 B. Females
Suicide by age 45−.0003−.0003−.0001
 (.0005)(.0005)(.0004)
Mental health–related death by age 45.0002.0002.0002
 (.0005)(.0005)(.0005)
Death by age 45.0007.0006.0005
 (.0011)(.0011)(.010)
MHD at ages 16–45.0029.0027.0029
 (.0019)(.0019)(.0020)
MHD at ages 26–35.0020.0019.0019
 (.0012)(.0012)(.0013)
MHD at ages 36–45.0012.0011.0013
 (.0014)(.0015)(.0016)
Depression at ages 16–45.0015.0014.0016
 (.0009)(.0010)(.0010)

Note.  We report the results only on outcomes parallel to those in table 7. MHD = Mental health disorder requiring hospitalization. Columns 2 and 3 present controlled direct effects based on Acharya, Blackwell, and Sen (2016). Education controls include the years of schooling and a dummy for having a high school degree. Income control is the log of taxable income at ages 26–35. Each cell reports the estimated effect of the comprehensive school reform from separate models. All models include cohort and municipality fixed effects. Standard errors, reported in parentheses, are clustered at the municipal level. In cols. 2 and 3, standard errors have been bootstrapped using 1,000 replications and clustered at the municipal level. Coefficients in italics survive a Romano-Wolf correction for multiple-hypothesis testing (Romano and Wolf 2005) at the 10% significance level (using estimates for full sample and the three subsamples of low, middle, and high parental education within gender).

* Significant at the 10% level (all two-sided tests).

** Significant at the 5% level (all two-sided tests).

View Table Image: 1 | 2

Appendix C.  Testing for Mechanisms

In section VI, we examine the extent to which posttreatment schooling and income mediate the effect of the comprehensive school reform on mental health–related hospitalizations (and deaths). A simply augmented regression model with posttreatment mediator variables can lead to biased estimates (see Acharya, Blackwell, and Sen 2016).

Acharya, Blackwell, and Sen (2016) apply a sequential procedure that consistently estimates the treatment effect while holding the values of potential mediators fixed. Adopting their approach, we estimate these average controlled direct effects (ACDEs) as follows (see table 7):

1.  Estimate an augmented model: yijt=α+ηj+τt+β·REFORMjt+δXiPost+ϵijt, where XiPost are additional posttreatment controls (i.e., the years of schooling and dummy for having a high school degree, and/or the log of taxable income at ages 26–35).

2.  Create a demediated outcome variable: y˜ijt=yijtδ^XiPost.

3.  Estimate a model for the demediated outcome: y˜ijt=α+ηj+τt+κ·REFORMjt+ϵijt, where κ is the ACDE of the reform.

Because the final estimation step contains a generated dependent variable (y˜ijt), the standard errors have been bootstrapped using 1,000 replications of the full process (steps 1–3). Additionally, the bootstrap replications have been clustered at the municipal level.

If the estimated ACDE is significantly different from zero, we can conclude that the comprehensive school reform has influenced mental health–related hospitalizations through pathways other than the education and income channel. By contrast, if the estimated ACDE is zero, then the reform has not had an additional effect on mental health–related hospitalizations once the proposed mechanisms have been accounted for. In other words, the reform effect would be exclusively driven by the mechanisms related to the changes in education and income.

Notes

We are thankful for the valuable comments from Jay Bhattacharya and Isaac Ehrlich (the editors), the three anonymous reviewers, Artturi Björk, Hannu Karhunen, Sari Kerr, Kristian Koerselman, Jani-Petri Laamanen, Krista Riukula, Kerstin Schneider, Tuomo Suhonen, the Annual Conference of the Royal Economic Society (2019), the European Association of Labour Economists (2018), the International Institute of Public Finance (2018), the Irish Economic Association (2018), seminar participants at the Queen’s University Belfast, the Economic and Social Research Institute, the Labour Institute for Economic Research, the University of Tampere, the University of Jyväskylä, and the University of Queensland. This study is part of a project supported by the Strategic Research Council at the Academy of Finland (project nos. 293120 and 314208), the Alfred Kordelin Foundation, the OP Group Research Foundation, and the Yrjö Jahnsson Foundation (no. 6621).

1 In our context of school systems in Europe, tracking refers to the streaming of students between the academic and vocational educational tracks, whereas in the United States tracking usually refers to ability grouping within schools (Hall 2012).

2 Nevertheless, age of tracking differs significantly among the OECD (Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development) countries (OECD 2004, 262).

3 Previous studies have used the reform to study non–mental health outcomes (Pekkarinen 2008; Pekkarinen, Uusitalo, and Pekkala Kerr 2009; Pekkala Kerr, Pekkarinen, and Uusitalo 2013).

4 Ability groups in foreign languages and mathematics existed in the comprehensive schools (grades 7–9) until 1985 (Sahlberg 2014, 28).

5 Schooling from age 7 to age 11 (grades 1–4) remained unchanged after the reform as the teachers from the prereform system were assigned to the comprehensive schools, and curricula in both systems were similar for grades 1–4 (Somerkivi 1982, 28).

6 Although the reform mostly proceeded from north to south, fig. B6 documents that in the adoption of the reform, substantial within-municipality variation existed among the four major socioeconomic regions.

7 The changes in peer composition could also affect individuals’ pool of potential partners and thus marriage outcomes, owing to assortative matching. We leave this issue for future studies.

8 In addition, the quality of health data before 1972 is weaker (Sund 2012, 507).

9 We followed individuals until age of 45 or their death, whichever came before, resulting in unbalanced panel data. Since mortality is one of the outcomes of interest to us, we did not condition on being in the panel until age 45.

10 In the original sample, between ages 11 and 16, 5.3% of individuals migrated between municipalities. Less than 2.6% migrated between the six waves (years) of adoption (see fig. 2). Only these latter individuals (47.3% of the migrants) were excluded from the estimation sample. We have investigated the potential role of endogenous selection of municipality of residence by assigning the individuals to treatment on the basis of their municipality of birth and date of birth, as in Meghir, Palme, and Simeonova (2018); see table B18 and discussion in sec. V.C.

11 The correlation between parents’ education and children’s academic ability is strong when the ability is measured by high school completion (see table B1). Also note that, contrary to the parental subsamples, the full sample includes a small number of individuals whose parental information is missing (less than 5%). However, the results remain intact if we exclude these individuals from the sample.

12 For Finnish men, in particular, accidental alcohol poisoning is the most prevalent category in a broader group of accidental poisonings. Accidental drug overdoses are very rare in Finland for the cohorts that we study.

13 Finnish outpatient data are available only for the most recent years, and the data are not nationally representative.

14 Most mental disorders emerge before the age of 25 (Pedersen et al. 2014).

15 The average duration of one spell, conditional on being hospitalized between ages 16 and 45, is 33 days for men and 38 days for women (the corresponding medians are 7 and 11 days).

16 We also estimate the models with six year-of-adoption dummies (see fig. 2) instead of the full set of 465 municipal dummies. We do this because previous studies using the Finnish comprehensive school reform had access to only the aggregated regional classification (i.e., six regional dummies), because of data limitations. The results are robust to this (see table B4, panel A). Panel B of table B4 provides estimation results based on 18 NUTS-3 (Nomenclature of Territorial Units for Statistics, level 3) regional dummies that also utilize within-region variation.

17 Consistent with Attanasio et al. (2017), our baseline Romano-Wolf correction uses four groups: the full sample and the three subsamples of low-, mid-, and highly educated parents. We perform the correction using these four groups, separately for males and females. The medical literature shows that the likelihood of mental health disorders differs substantially by gender (Salk, Hyde, and Abramson 2017), and thus analyses are separately done for both genders. We also implement the procedure with three groups, i.e., low-, mid-, and highly educated parents, or six groups, i.e., (male, female) × (low-, mid-, highly educated parents). Results of this alternative multiple-hypothesis testing are reported in table B12.

18 The results are also robust to using additional birth month dummies or birth month–by–birth year dummies (table B5), as estimated by Lager et al. (2016).

19 Furthermore, fig. B5 shows that at ages 6–10 there is no evidence for significant effects on mental health–related hospitalizations. However, this test is relatively weak, since it is possible that the replacement of the old system (primary schools) with comprehensive school system also affected 7–10-year-old children, even though the curriculum remained largely unchanged (Somerkivi 1982, 28).

20 The aggregate results for both genders together are reported in table B5. None of the estimated effects for suicides or all-cause mortality are significant at the standard 5% level. The reform effect on mental health–related deaths is negative (p<.05) for individuals from low-educated families.

21 For females, the null effects are consistent with those of the earlier Finnish study of Ravesteijn et al. (2017). Using an 11% random sample of the population, Ravesteijn et al. (2017) find longevity gains (losses) for males from low-income (high-income) families.

References